POSITIVE INK: The positive in "Disease Positive"
I've always bee...: The positive in "Disease Positive" I've always been somewhat fascinated by the concept of looking at the positive side of ...
Friday, November 11, 2016
Thursday, July 28, 2016
Figure-Ground Perception
Consolidated by
Akshata
Jayant Chonkar ( 1630221)
I CEP-B
Christ University, Bangalore
Figure-ground
perception has evolved from the Gestalt school of thought. Figure–ground
organization is a type of perceptual grouping which is a vital necessity for
recognizing objects through vision.
Perceptual
organization is a construct of the Gestalt School of thought. Gestalt theory
was first developed in the early 1900s by Austrian and German psychologists.
Some of the notable founders of Gestalt theory include Max Wertheimer, and Kurt
Koffka. One of the main beliefs of Gestalt theory is that a something as a
whole consists of interacting parts that can be separated, analyzed, and
rearranged in the whole. In other words, the whole is different from the sum of
its parts. Max Wertheimer and scientists with him believed that humans have an
inborn tendency to construct meaningful perceptions from fragments of sensory
input. This theory of perception proposes that people make sense of the world
around them by taking separate and distinct elements and combining them into a
unified whole. For example, if you look at shapes drawn on a piece of paper,
your mind will likely group the shapes in terms of things such as similarity or
proximity.
People
automatically focus on some objects in the perceptual field to the exclusion of
others. What we focus on is the figure, and what fades away is the the
background.
When a
person sees buildings, cars, trees, people, etc.- all these objects are
perceived as figures in front of backgrounds of the sky, or other buildings,
etc. When figure-ground relationships are ambiguous, or capable of being
interpreted in various ways, our perceptions tend to be unstable, shifting back
and forth. An example of this would be a reversible figure, which is a drawing
that one can perceive in different ways by reversing the figure and ground. In
some examples, a shift occurs in our perceptions of what is figure and what is
ground.
Psychologists
have created different kinds of stimuli in order to study how people separate
figure from ground. The interpretations that people derive from these stimuli
are real, even though the objects are ambiguous or are nonexistent.
The
"faces or vases" illustration is one of the most frequent
demonstrations of figure-ground. What you see depends on whether you see the
white as the figure or the black as the figure.
If you see the white as the figure, then you
perceive a vase. If you see the black as the figure, then you see two faces in
profile. Most people are able to reverse their perceptions and switch back and
forth between the vase and faces images.
When
looking at a visual scene, people tend to look for ways to differentiate
between the figure and the ground. Some ways that people accomplish this
include:
Blurriness: Objects in the foreground tend to
be crisp and distinct while those in the background are blurry or hazy.
Contrast: High contrast between objects can
lead to the perception of figure and ground The Rubin vase is one example.
Size: Images that appear to be larger
will be perceived as closer and part of the figure while those that are smaller
will seem further away and part of the background.
Separation: An object isolated from everything
else in a visual scene is more likely to be seen as a figure versus background.
Law of common fate: Elements seen moving together are perceived as
belonging together
All
of these laws help us understand how people transform raw visual inputs into
meaningful displays
Bibliography
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Figure–ground_(perception
http://study.com/academy/lesson/figure-ground-perception-definition-examples.html
https://www.verywell.com/what-is-figure-ground-perception-2795195
Introduction to Psychology,
Clifford T. Morgan, Richard King; 7th Edition; McGraw Hill Educatin Pvt Ltd
Psychology- Robert A Baron; 5th
edition; Pearson Publications
Psychology: Concepts and
Connections- Spencer A. Rathus ; 7th Edition; Thomson and Wadsworth
Psychology- Saul Kassin; 2nd
Edition; Hall
Tuesday, July 26, 2016
BASIC CONCEPTS OF SENSATION
Consolidated by
Abhishek Singh Chauhan, 1630201, I CEP B
Christ University, Bangalore, Karnataka
As defined by
APA Glossary of Psychological Terms, sensation is the process by which
stimulation of a sensory receptor gives rise to neural impulses that result in
an experience, or awareness of, conditions inside or outside the body.
In simpler words
sensation can be explained as the process by which our sense organs receive
inputs from our environment. Hence, the classical human senses – sight, sound,
taste, smell and touch– register the sensory inputs that evoke our respective
sense organs. But it is important to note that the scope of sensory experience
is much wider than the basic functions of these five senses, as sensory
information can also be in the form of light, pressure, vibration, heat and pain.
These are the stimuli to which our nervous system reacts through sensory
receptors.
A stimulus
is a physical influence that initiates and provokes a reaction. It can be
specific information that is received by the sensory organs, and the resulted
experience is a sensation. Stimuli can vary in type and intensity. Different
sense organs respond to different stimuli, and the intensity of the stimulus
decides whether it can be deducted by the sensory receptors. This is the core
subject matter of the branch of psychology called psychophysics, which studies the physical characteristics of stimuli
in relation with our psychological reaction to them.
Sensory
receptors are the specialized structures that
react to a physical stimulus. A group of receptor cells is called a sense
organ. These receptors vary in classification –taste, gustatory, odour,
olfactory, etc. – but perform the same basic function of being in contact with
the physical stimulus to receive information, decode this information into
creating neural impulses or signals, and transmit these impulses to sensory
cortices of the brain for it to interpret them. This interpretation is called perception.
Sensation and perception are often explained
as one unit. These are two interrelated but distinct parts of a continuous
process. Sensation is a physical response of detecting and translating the
sensory information into signals going up to the brain (bottom-up processing),
while perception is a psychological
response that follows it by analysing and interpreting these signals and makes
sense of them based on our experiences and expectation (top-down processing).
A stimulus has
to be strong enough to be detected by the sense organs. For that, it has to
have the smallest level of intensity to be registered by our senses, which is
called an absolute threshold. However, at such low levels, it may
be difficult to detect a stimulus if noise (any disturbance or distraction in
the form of background stimulation that interferes with other senses) is present
in the environment. Therefore, an absolute threshold is defined as a level of
stimulus that is detectable 50 percent of the time. These thresholds may vary
with age.
The minimum
change in stimulus intensity required to detect the difference in sensory
experience 50 percent of the time is called difference
threshold or a just noticeable
difference. Ernest Weber observed that the size of difference threshold is
a constant proportion of the original stimulus magnitude. This relation is
called Weber’s Law and is expressed
as ∆I / I = K, where ∆I represents the difference threshold, I represents the
initial stimulus intensity and K signifies the proportion that remains constant
despite changes in I. For examples, if two cell-phones are playing music with
the intensity of 100 units, and one’s volume is increased just enough for it to
be noticeably louder, the increased volume would have the intensity of 110
units, and the just noticeable difference would be 10. The Weber fraction in
this would be 10/100 = 0.1. This being a constant proportion, a person’s
difference threshold can be predicted for any level of intensity using the law.
Due to repeated
exposure to a stimulus, our sensitivity to it decreases. This happens through sensory adaptation, which is an adjustment our brain makes to accustom
itself to an unchanging stimulus. This happens when a strong odour in a room
can no longer be smelled after a while. It is not because the smell vanishes,
but our nerve cells fire less frequently in response to the continuous
exposure, and we adapt to it.
BIBLIOGRAPHY:
· Feldman, Robert S.
(1986) Essentials of Understanding Psychology. New York City, NY, USA:
McGraw-Hill Education. (pp. 91-96)
· Myers, David G. (2009)
Psychology – 9th edition in modules. New York City, NY, USA: Worth Publishers.
(pp. 225-231)
Glossary of
Psychological Terms – American Psychological Association.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)